Taylor and Francis Group is part of the Academic Publishing Division of Informa PLC

This site is operated by a business or businesses owned by Informa PLC and all copyright resides with them. Informa PLC's registered office is 5 Howick Place, London SW1P 1WG. Registered in England and Wales. Number 3099067.

Informa

Unit 5. Implicature: (re)assessing the utility of the CP

In A5.1.3, we outlined the CP and its attendant Maxims (Quality, Quantity, Relation, Manner), and went on to detail the different ways of breaking these Maxims (in A5.1.4). In A5.2, we highlighted some of the issues that Grice himself raised in regard to the CP: they included his recognition that, potentially, there are ‘all sorts of other maxims ... such as “Be polite”’ (Grice 1975: 47). The remainder of A5 was then devoted to a discussion of how Neo-Griceans and Post-Griceans have sought to modify – and, in some cases, revolutionize – the Gricean approach to implicature, as a means of eradicating some of its ‘inherent problems’.

In this section, we want you to have the chance to test the utility of the CP for yourself. We begin by asking you whether Gricean implicature is indicative of linguistic or social cooperation. We then provide examples of how researchers have used the CP (and its Maxims) in conjunction with another procedural concept – specifically, activity types(Levinson 1979, 1992), metapragmatic framing strategies (Janney 2007) and reality paradigms (Archer 2002) – in order to be able to explain conflictive interaction: and you are given opportunities to test these methods.

Read more...

  • Archer, D. (2002) ‘“Can innocent people be guilty?” A sociopragmatic analysis of examination transcripts from the Salem Witchcraft Trials’, Journal of Historical Pragmatics 3(1): 1–30
  • Grice, H.P. (1975) ‘Logic and conversation’, in P. Cole and J. Morgan (eds) Syntax and semantics 3: speech acts, pp. 41–58. New York: Academic Press. Reprinted in Grice 1989: 22–57
  • Janney, R.W. (2007) ‘“So your story now is that...”: Metapragmatic framing strategies in courtroom interrogation’. In: W. Bublitz and A. Hübler (eds.) Metapragmatics in Use, pp223–34. Amsterdam and Philadelphia: John Benjamins
  • Levinson, S.C. (1979) ‘Activity types and language’ Linguistics 17(5-6): 356–99
  • Levinson, S.C. (1992) ‘Activity types and language’ in P. Drew and  J. Heritage (eds.) Talk at Work: Interaction in Insitutional Settings, Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, pp66–100

5.1. Gricean implicature – indicative of linguistic or social cooperation?

Our focus on conflictive interaction necessitates that we also reintroduce here an issue that we briefly outlined in C6.3: whether the CP, as Grice defined it, was meant to capture linguistic cooperation or social cooperation.

Our position is similar to that of Bousfield (2008: 27), namely that Grice had in mind linguisticcooperation, not least because:

Immediately following his own definition of the CP; the maxims of Quantity, Quality, Relation and Manner, Grice says that, ‘There are, of course, all sorts of other maxims (aesthetic, social or moral in character)’ (Grice 1975: 47...). Now, it could be argued that had Grice intended his CP to be a model of social cooperation (and his maxims, therefore, as being socially directed maxims), then he would not have indicated ‘social’ maxims as being an ‘other’ type of maxim to the ones he himself had just stipulated for the CP; its categories and subordinate maxims.

What ‘really confirms Grice’s position’, for Bousfield (2008: 27), however, is Grice’s explicit indication that he considered – but then abandoned – the idea of the CP as a possible system of ‘social goal sharing’. Here, Bousfield has in mind Grice’s explanation of the way in which our expectations for social cooperation can grow as an interaction develops – to the point of becoming ‘quasi-contractual’. Yet, Grice nonetheless signalled that, even when a common aim might be evident, ‘like getting a car mended’, it is still possible that ‘the ultimate aims’ of the participants ‘may ... be independent and even in conflict – each may want to get the car mended in order to drive off, leaving the other stranded’, for example (Grice 1989: 29).

Mooney (2004: 902) also believes the CP can be ‘at work’ in socially uncooperative/conflictive contexts. But she believes the CP could be more powerful, interpretatively speaking, if ‘cooperation’ was linked much more explicitly to the notion of activity types (Levinson 1979, 1992). By way of illustration:

the activity type that is a cross-examination tells participants what is and what is not required and allowed. It is also embedded in the conventions of the courtroom ... Using activity types forces the analyst to reconsider the goals of [such] discourse activities ... The benefit of adopting the activity type in conjunction with the CP is [that i]t contextualizes the maxims and allows a clearer and more relevant construal of contributions. While it is not predictive in a detailed way, it certainly serves to make Gricean analysis more powerful and explicable ... (Mooney 2004: 905)

TASK

According to Mooney (2004: 905), ‘until one knows the “structural properties of an activity” one cannot understand the way in which these properties “constrain ... the verbal contributions that are made” (Levinson 1979: 370)’. As Mooney’s reference to Levinson makes clear, she is alluding to his definition of an activity type as:

a fuzzy category whose focal members are goal-defined, socially constituted, bounded, events with constraints on participants, setting, and so on, but above all on the kinds of allowable contributions. (1979: 370, 1992: 69)

Investigate a courtroom transcript1 as a means of familiarizing yourself with the structural properties of this activity type, noting in particular what types of verbal contributions are allowed, by whom they are used, and the goals of these different participants.

In A5.1.4 and A12.5, we suggested that the Quality Maxim is probably suspended in an interrogation/police interview. Do you think this is also true of the courtroom? Or is it more likely that participants opt to signal their inability to cooperate in the way the Quality Maxim ideally requires (cf. Grice’s definition for opt outs and/or clash(es))? Where possible, draw from your courtroom transcript to justify your answer.

How might the Quantity, Manner and Relation Maxims be affected by the courtroom procedure? Again, where possible, draw from your courtroom transcript to justify your answer.

Read more...

  • Bousfield, D. (2008) Impoliteness in Interaction. Amsterdam and Philadelphia: John Benjamins (especially Section 2.1.1.2, ‘Grice: As social cooperation or linguistic cooperation?’ pp25–9)
  • Grice, H.P. (1989) Studies in the way of words. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press
  • Levinson, S.C. (1979) ‘Activity types and language’ Linguistics 17(5-6): 356–99
  • Levinson, S.C. (1992) ‘Activity types and language’ in Drew, P. and J. Heritage (eds.) Talk at Work: Interaction in Insitutional Settings, Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, pp66–100
  • Mooney, A. (2004) ‘Co-operation, violations and making sense’, Journal of Pragmatics 36: 899–920

1 Some American trials are available in full via the internet. In the UK it is more common for public inquiries to be made publicly available. The following link, for example, provides a range of data relating to the Shipman Inquiry: http://www.shipman-inquiry.org.uk